The Lower Row Monkey Cage: An Overlooked Variable in Biomedical Research

Viktor Reinhardt and Annie Reinhardt
Animal Welfare Institute
Washington, DC

A survey of 96 primatological articles revealed that cage location of research monkeys is rarely mentioned, although the environment of upper and lower row-housed animals markedly differs in terms of light quality, light intensity, and living dimension. Not accounting for these uncontrolled variables may increase variability of data and, consequently, the number of experimental animals needed to obtain statistically acceptable results. This study concluded that single-tier housing would be an important refinement of research methodology. Such housing would (a) enable all animals of a room to use the "arboreal" dimension of their enclosure and retreat to "safe" vantage points above the human "predator," (b) offer all animals access to uniform light, and (c) provide more favorable conditions for professional animal care.

Cages of small and medium size nonhuman primates are commonly arranged in double tiers with one row stacked on top of the other (Rosenberg & Kesel, 1994). This housing is financially attractive, but it carries with it serious ethical and methodological implications.

Monkeys are biologically adapted to withdraw in alarming situations and avoid terrestrial predators by taking to the arboreal—vertical—dimension of their environment (Chopra, Seth, & Seth, 1992; DeVore & Hall, 1965; Home Office, 1989; Lindburg, 1971; Roonwal & Mohnot, 1977). The presence of cliffs, trees, or other high structures for refuge from predators affects the distribution of monkeys more than the edibility of the vegetation does (Jay, 1965; Hamilton, 1982; Simonds, 1965): "Even macaques, which [sic] some describe as semiterrestrial, spend most of the day in elevated locations and seek the refuge of sleeping trees at night" (National Research Council, 1998, p. 92; cf. Lindburg, 1971; Roonwal & Mohnot, 1977; Wheatley, 1980).

The placement of the perch does not allow the space underneath it to be comfortably occupied.
Figure 1. The standard lower tier cage provides a crepuscular, cavelike living environment, which not only jeopardizes the animals' well-being but also introduces uncontrolled extraneous variables into scientific data collected from upper row- and lower row-caged research subjects.

In the conventional double-tier system, monkeys in lower rows—unlike those in upper rows—are forced to lead a terrestrial lifestyle. They will sit on perches for a considerable portion of the day (Reinhardt, 1989; Shimoji, Bowers, & Crockett, 1993; Woodbeck & Reinhardt, 1991) to get at least a short distance away from the "unsafe" horizontal dimension of the room (see Figure 1). But even with access to perches, lower row-caged animals are unable to retreat to a safe place above the human predator who periodically captures them and subjects them to both life-threatening situations (Fowler, 1995, Figures 8-11, 19-22; Klein & Murray, 1995, Figures 1,7, 10, 11) and distressing procedures (Fuller, Hobson, Reyes, Winter, & Faiman, 1984; Ives & Dack, 1956; Line, Clarke, & Markowitz, 1987; Line, Morgan, Markowitz, & Strong, 1989; Mason, 1972; Mason, Mougey, & Kenion, 1973; Puri, Puri, & Anand-Kumar, 1981; Schnell & Gerber, 1997). No wonder that caged monkeys show behavioral and physiological fear responses when someone dressed in protective garb enters the room and approaches their cages. Their responses include alarm vocalization, acute diarrhea, fear grinning, fear threatening, crouching in a far corner, stereotypical locomotion, self-biting, increased heart rate, elevated blood pressure, raised core temperature, and pronounced cardiac reactivity (Bowers, Crockett, & Bowden, 1998; Draper & Bernstein, 1963; Malinow, Hill, & Ochsner, 1974; Reinhardt, 1994, 1999b; Reinhardt & Seelig, 1998; Schnell & Wood, 1993).

The situation must be especially distressing for lower row-caged monkeys who "might perceive the presence of humans above them as particularly threatening" (National Research Council, 1998, p. 118). Having no vertical flight option, they are quasi cornered and might experience intensive anxiety in the presence of the looming human predator.

Lower row housing per se does not imply that the animals "suffer behaviorally" (Schapiro, Stavisky, & Hook, 2000, p. 5) but that lower row-caged subjects are likely to be more disturbed ( e.g., more intensive fear behavior; higher blood pressure; higher respiration rate; higher levels of norepinephrine, prolactin, and cortisol; higher white blood cell count; Lilly, Mehlman, & Higley, 1999; Van der Kar, Richardson-Morton, & Rittenhouse, 1991) by the presence of fear-inducing personnel than upper row-caged subjects. It is this difference in the acute fear response prior to the actual experiment that introduces an uncontrolled variable, thereby diminishing the validity of subsequently collected research data. The National Research Council (1998) alluded to this problem in its book The Psychological Well-Being of Nonhuman Primates, advising that primates should not be housed in double-tier cages. Unfortunately, this advice refers only to marmosets and fails to include other primates who are equally dependent on access to high, safe locations.

The majority of nonhuman primates used in biomedical research are diurnal animals. In the traditional double-tier system, however, monkeys of the bottom row are forced to live in the crepuscular shade area of the upper row (see Figure 1). The indirect light they receive is reflected and its spectral distribution altered, depending on the colors of the surrounding walls. Because cage illumination often is poor, a flashlight is needed to identify and inspect the cage occupants correctly—tattoos and general health check, respectively— as well as to properly illuminate the cage interior (e.g., waste material in back corners of cage) and the drop pan (e.g., feces indicative of shigellosis; Reinhardt, 1999a). Inadequate animal care and insufficient cage hygiene often result. The National Research Council (1996) addressed this dilemma in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, stipulating that lighting should provide sufficient illumination to allow good housekeeping practices and adequate inspection of animals, "including the bottom-most cages in racks" (p. 34). The United States Department of Agriculture (1999) made a similar observation and suggested that "it is also helpful to redesign rooms so primates are not kept in darkened lower-tier cages" (p. 43). Unfortunately, there are no recommendations for accomplishing this (Reinhardt, 1997).

That lower row-caged monkeys live in a much darker environment than those kept in upper rows is no secret but has been documented repeatedly—although without intention— in the scientific literature (Abee, 1985, Figure 1; Anonymous, 1973, Figure 2; Coid, 1967, Figure 1; Gay, 1957, Figure 3; Gengozian, 1969, Figure 5; Bonjo, 1985, Figure 2; Kemnitz, 1980, Figure 1; King &Norwood, 1989, Figure 1; Klein & Murray, 1995, Figure 7; Milhaud, Klein, & Merkel, 1980, Figure 2; Ohto, 1993, Figure 1; Valerio et al., 1969, Figure 18; Weed, Baker, Barbaugh, & Erwin, 1995, Figure 2). A recent study demonstrates a statistically significant difference (p < .001) in mean light levels of upper row and bottom row monkey cages at nine different positions within the cage (Schapiro et al., 2000). This observation validates the notion that "intensity of light in animal cages is likely to be the most variable environmental factor in the average animal room" (Clough, 1982, p. 512).

The influence of direct versus indirect light on biomedical parameters has not yet received any attention in the primatological literature. A few publications evaluate the impact of light intensity. Isaac and DeVito (1958) and Draper (1965) reported that decreased illumination is accompanied by decreased activity in single-caged rhesus macaques. Schapiro et al. (2000) scored a more than two-fold higher incidence of abnormal behaviors and a more than five-fold higher level of inactivity in six long-tailed macaques housed in relatively dark lower row cages than in three long-tailed macaques housed in relatively bright upper row cages; the comparison did not reach statistical significance, probably because of the small sample size. Scott (1991) observed that locomotor activity is lower in pairs of marmosets kept in dark lower tier cages than in pairs kept in bright upper tier cages. Box and Rohrhuber (1993) noted that pairs of tamarins housed in (presumed to be relatively dark) lower tier cages engage in less amicable behavior and are less active than pairs housed in upper tiers. Beger, Merker, and Neubert (1986) underscored that pair-housed female marmosets excrete abnormally low levels of steroid hormones and show prolonged ovulatory cycles associated with substantially reduced fecundity when kept in dim lower row cages rather than in bright upper row cages.

These findings indicate that illumination is an important variable that both may influence animal well-being (National Research Council, 1996; United States Department of Agriculture, 1991) and potentially decrease research quality (Bellhom, 1980; Clough, 1982; Mulder, 1971; Ott, 1974). The International Primatological Society (1989) and the United States Department of Agriculture (1991) aptly stipulated that illumination should be not only sufficient but also uniform.

To bend or kneel down while inspecting monkeys in lower tiers is less convenient than standing in an upright position while checking monkeys in upper tiers. Consequently, monkeys in lower rows tend to receive less attention by animal care personnel (e.g., Ross & Everitt, 1988). The daily check of both their well-being and the cleanliness of their cages may be so haphazard that developing health and hygiene problems go unnoticed.

Scientific investigators should be concerned about the traditional lower row cage arrangement because it not only jeopardizes the research subjects' well-being but could also affect the validity of research data collected from them.

METHOD

A survey was conducted of all scientific articles published since inception in the Journal of Medical Primatology {Vols. 1-28) and the Journal of American Primatology {Vols. 1-48). These two journals were chosen because it was assumed that primatologists are more knowledgeable than other scientific professionals of the characteristic needs of nonhuman primates. Investigations—dealing with possible stress-sensitive or illumination-sensitive parameters and using small or medium size primates in cages—were screened for descriptions of the research subjects' cage location.

RESULTS

We examined 96 articles that investigated reproduction (53), stress (20), blood composition (11), behavior (6), and body growth (3) in macaques (75), vervets (7), marmosets (5), squirrel monkeys (3), capuchin monkeys (3), tamarins (1), mangabeys (1), and patas monkeys (1).

Cage location is mentioned in 2 of the 96 articles but not in the remaining 94 (see Table 1). Description of the research subjects' housing conditions usually follows a cliche formula such as " All animals were housed in standard cages in a controlled environment."

DISCUSSION

Only 2% of the scientific articles surveyed take notice of the research subjects' cage location, and 98% make no reference to whether all or some research subjects were housed in upper row, lower row, or single row cages. This omission suggests that "most investigators think only briefly about the care and handling of their animals" (Traystman, 1987, p. 108). Therefore, they presumably are not aware of the gross differences in the microenvironment of upper versus lower row-caged primates. Not accounting for these uncontrolled variables may increase data variability and, consequently, the number of experimental animals needed to obtain statistically acceptable results. It also makes it problematic, if not impossible, to either reliably test research findings in another laboratory or compare the findings with those of another laboratory. Research data of different laboratories will vary, resulting in discrepancies in the scientific literature.

TABLE 1
Survey of Scientific Articles Published in Two Primatological Journals

Research Field No. of Articles Surveyed Location of Cage Is Mentioned Location of Cage Is Not Mentioned
Reproductiona 56 0 56
Stressb 20 1 19
Blood compositionc 11 0 11
Behaviord 6 1 5
Body growth 3 0 3
Total 96 (100%) 2 (2%) 94 (98%)

aEstrogen, progesterone, testosterone, FSH, LH, reproductive performance. bCortisol, ACTH, prolactin, heart rate, immune response. cHemogram, serum chemistry. dAbnormal behavior, response to environmental enrichment.

From the statistical point of view, to overlook the variable "cage location" would be acceptable only if it has been demonstrated first that upper and lower row housing do not influence the physiological and behavioral responsiveness of the research subjects during the experiment. As far as we know, this has never been done. Yet, it is taken for granted that research monkeys live in a strictly controlled microenvironment.

There are two solutions to problems associated with the double-tier system:

  1. Monkeys are housed in single tiers in well-structured cages (perches or shelves at different levels) that reach close to the ceiling of the room. This assures that all cages of a room are directly and equally illuminated, and that all animals of a room have equal opportunities to access the arboreal dimension of their environment and to retreat to safe vantage points above human eye level (cf. International Primatological Society, 1993).
  2. Editors of scientific journals should require that authors describe the caging arrangement of research monkeys in such detail that the investigation can be duplicated reliably in another laboratory (cf. Canadian Council on Animal Care, 1993; Davis et al., 1973).

REFERENCES

Abee, C. R. (1985). Medical care and management of the squirrel monkey. In L. A. Rosenblum & C. L. Coe (Eds.), Handbook of squirrel monkey research (pp. 447-488). New York: Plenum.

Anonymous. (1973). Primate breeding on a grand scale. Lab Animal, 2(3), 16-18, 53.

Bellhorn, R W. (1980). Lighting in the animal environment. Laboratory Animal Science, 30, 440-450. Bowers, C. L., Crockett, C. M., & Bowden, D. M. (1998). Differences in stress reactivity of laboratory macaques measured by heart period and respiratory sinus arrhythmia. American Journal of Primatology, 45, 245-261.

Box, H. 0., & Rohrhuber, B. (1993). Differences in behaviour among adult male/female pairs of cottontop tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) in different conditions of housing. Animal Technology, 44. 19-30.

Chopra, P. K., Seth, P. K. & Seth, S. (1992). Behavioural profile of free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Primate Report, 32, 75-105.

Clough, G. (1982). Environmental effects on animals used in biomedical research. Biological Reviews, 57, 487-523.

Coid, C. R. (1967). A system of caging monkeys. Laboratory Animals, 1, 25-29.

Davis, D. E., Bennett, E. L., Berkson, G., Lang, C. M., Snyder, R L., & Pick, J. R (1973). Recommendations for a standardized minimum description of animal treatment. Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources News, 16(4), 3-4.

DeVore, I., & Hall, K. R. L. (1965). Baboon ecology. In I. DeVore (Ed.), Primate behavior—Field studies of monkeys and apes (pp. 20-52). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Draper, W. A. (1965). Sensory stimulation and rhesus monkey activity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 21, 319-322.

Draper, W. A., & Bernstein, I. S. (1963). Stereotyped behavior and cage size. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 16, 231-234.

Fowler, M. E. (1995). Restraint and handling of wild and domestic animals (2nd ed.). Ames: Iowa State University Press.

Fuller, G. B., Hobson, W. C., Reyes,F. I., Winter, I. S. D., & Faiman, C. (1984). lnfluence of restraint and ketamine anesthesia on adrenal steroids, progesterone, and gonadotropins in rhesus monkeys. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 175, 487-490.

Gay, W. I. (1957). Husbandry practices for the quarantining and conditioning of sub-human primates. Proceedings of the Animal Care Panel, 7. 83-88.

Gengozian, N. (1969). Marmosets: Their potential in experimental medicine. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 162, 336-362.

Hamilton, W. J. (1982). Baboon sleeping site preferences and relationships to primate grouping patterns. American Journal of Primatology, 3, 41-53.

Heger, W., Merker,H.-I., & Neubert, D. (1986). Low light intensity decreases the fertility of Callithrix jacchus. Primate Report, 14, 260.

Home Office. (1989). Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986. Code of practice for the housing and care of animals used in scientific procedures. London: Her Majesty's Stationery Office.

Honjo, S. (1985). The Japanese Tsukuba Primate Center for Medical Science (TPC): An outline. Journal of Medical Primatology. 14, 75-89.

International Primatological Society. (1989). International guidelines for the acquisition, care and breeding of nonhuman primates. Primate Report, 25, 3-27.

International Primatological Society. (1993). International guidelines for the acquisition, care and breeding of nonhuman primates: Codes of practice 1-3. Primate Report, 35, 3-29.

Isaac, W., & DeVito, J. L. (1958). Effect of sensory stimulation on the activity of normal and prefrontal-labectomized monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 51, 172-174.

Ives, M., & Dack, G. M. (1956). "Alarm reaction" and normal blood picture in Macaca mulatta. Journal of Laboratory Clinical Medicine, 47, 723-729.

Jay, P. (1965). The common langur of North India. In I. DeVore (Ed.), Primate behavior——Field studies of monkeys and apes (pp. 197-249). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Kemnitz, J. (1980). Various factors influence feeding behavior, obesity. Primate Record. 8(2), 3-5.

King, J. E., & Norwood, V. R (1989). Free-environment rooms as altenative housing for squirrel monkeys. In E. F. Sega1 (Ed.), Housing, care and psychogical well-being of captive and laboratory primates (pp. 102-114). Park Ridge, NJ: Noyes Data.

Klein, H. J., & Murray, K. A. (1995). Restraint. In B. T. Bennett, C. R Abee, & R Henrickson (Eds.), Nonhuman primates in biomedical research——biology and management (pp. 286-297). New York: Academic.

Lilly, A., Mehlman, P. T., & Hig1ey, J. D. (1999). Trait-like immunological and hematological measures in female rhesus across varied environmental conditions. American Journal of Primatology, 48, 197-223.

Lindburg, D. G. (1971). The rhesus monkey in North India: An ecological and behavioral study. In L. A. Rosenblum (Ed.), Primate behavior: Developments infield and laboratory research, Volume 2 (pp. 1-106). New York: Academic.

Line, S. W., Clarke, A. S.,& Markowitz, H. (1987, October). Plasma cortisol of female rhesus monkeys in response to acute restraint. Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 26(4),1-3.

Line, S. W., Morgan, K. N., Markowitz, H., & Strong, S. (1989, April). Heart rate and activity of rhesus monkeys in response to routine events. Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 28(2), 9-12.

Malinow, M. R., Hill, J. D., & Ochsner, A. J. (1974). Heart rate in caged rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Laboratory Anima/ Science, 24, 537-540.

Mason, J. W. (1972). Corticosteroid response to chair restraint in the monkey. American Journal of Physiology, 222, 1291-1294.

Mason, J. W., Mougey, E. H., & Kenion, C. C. (1973). Urinary epinephrine and norepinephrine responses to chair restraint in the monkey. Endocrinology and Behavior, 10, 811-804.

Milhaud, C. L., Klein, M. J., & Merkel, M. C. (1980). A new restraining chair for rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Medical Primatology, 9, 62-70.

Mulder, J. B. (1971). Animal behavior and electromagnetic energy waves. Laboratory Animal Science, 21, 389-393.

National Research Council. (1996). Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals (7th ed.). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

National Research Council. (1998). The psychological well-being of nonhuman primates. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

Ohto, O. (1993). From the front of animal feeding: Excrements, feces, and Tsukuba Primate Center News, 12(2), 30.

Olfert, B. M.,Cross, M.,& McWilliam, A. (Eds.). (1993). Guide to the care and use of experimental animals (Vol. 1, 2nd ed.). Ottawa, Canada: Canadian Council on Animal Care.

Ott, J. N. (1974). The importance of laboratory lighting as an experimental variable. In H. Magalhaes (Ed.), Environmental variables in animal experimentation (pp. 39-57). Lewisburg, PA: Bucknell University Press.

Purl, C. P., Purl, V.,& Anand-Kumar, T. C. (1981). Serum levels of testosterone, cortisol, prolactin and bioactive luteinizing hormone in adult male rhesus monkeys following cage-restraint or anaesthetizing with ketamine hydrochloride. Acta Endocrinologica, 97, 118-124.

Reinhardt, V. (1989). Evaluation of the long-term effectiveness of two environmental enrichment objects for singly caged rhesus macaques. Lab Animal, 18(6),31-33.

Reinhardt, V. (1994). Traditional handling procedures of laboratory nonhuman primates are an intrinsic source of distress: What can be done? In Touch, 1(4), 1, 6-7.

Reinhardt, V. (1997, Summer). Lighting conditions for laboratory monkeys: Are they adequate? Animal Welfare Information Center Newsletter, 8(2), 3-6.

Reinhardt, V. (1999a, July). The monkey cave: The dark lower row cage. Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 38(3), 8-9.

Reinhardt, V. ( 1999b, January). Pair-housing overcomes self-biting behavior in macaques. Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 38( I ), 4.

Reinhardt, V., & Seelig, D. (1998). Environmental enhancement for caged rhesus macaques: A photographic documentation. Washington, DC: Animal Welfare Institute.

Roonwal, M. L., & Mohnot, S. M. (1977). Primates of South Asia——Ecology, sociobiology, and behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Rosenberg, D. P ., & Kesel, M. L. ( 1994). Old-world monkeys. In B. E. Rollin & M. L. Kesel (Eds.), The experimental animal in biomedical research. Volume II: Care, husbandry, and well-being——An overview by species (pp. 457-483). Boca Raton, TX: CPR Press.

Ross, W. R, & Everitt, I. I. (1988). A nylon ball device for primate environmental enrichment. Laboratory Animal Science, 38, 481-483.

Schapiro, S. J., Stavisky, R.,& Hook, M. (2000, January). The lower row cage may be dark, but behavior does not appear to be affected. Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 39(1), 4-6.

Schnell, C. R., & Gerber, P. (1997). Training and remote monitoring of cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates. Primate Report, 49,61-70.

Schnell, C. R, & Wood, J. M. ( 1993). Measurement of blood pressure, heart rate, body temperature, ECG and activity by telemetry in conscious unrestrained marmosets. Proceedings of the Fifth Federation of European Laboratory Animal Science Association Symposium on Welfare and Science, 5, 107-111.

Scott, L. (1991). Environmental enrichment for single housed common marmosets. In H. 0. Box (Ed.), Primate responses to environmental change (pp. 265-274). London: Chapman & Hall.

Shimoji, M., Bowers, C. L., & Crockett, C. M. (1993, October). Initial response to introduction of a PVC perch by singly caged macaca fascicularisLaboratory Primate Newsletter, 32(4), 8-11.

Simonds, P. E. (1965). The bonnet macaque in South India. In I. DeVore (Ed.), Primate behavior——field studies of monkeys and apes (pp. 175-196). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Traystman, R. J. (1987). ACUC, who needs it? The investigator's viewpoint [Special issue]. Laboratory Animal Science, 37, 108-110.

United States Department of Agriculture. (1991). Title 9, CFR(Code of Federal Register), Part 3: Animal welfare, standards, final rule. Federal Register, 56, 6426--6505.

United States Department of Agriculture. ( 1999). Final report on environmental enhancement to pro- mote the psychological well-being of nonhuman primates. Riverdale, MD: Author.

Valerio, D. A., Miller, R. L., Innes, J. R. M., Courntey, K. D., Pallotta, A. J., & Guttmacher, R. M. (1969). Macaca mulatta——Management of a laboratory breeding colony. New York: Academic.

Van derKar, L. D., Richardson-Morton, K. D., & Rittenhouse, P. A. (1991). Stress: Neuroendocrine and pharmacological mechanism. In G. Jasmin & M. Cantin (Eds.), Stress revisited: Neuroendocrinology of stress: Vol. 14 (pp. 133-173). Basel, Switzerland: Karger.

Weed J. L., Baker, S. C., Harbaugh, S. W., & Erwin, J. (1995). Innovative enclosures for laboratory primates: Evaluation of a "breeding condominium." Lab Animal, 24(7), 28-32.

Wheatley, B. P. (1980). Feeding and ranging of East Bomean Macaca fascicularis. In D. G. Lindburg (Ed.), The macaques: Studies in ecology, behavior and evolution (pp. 215-246). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

Woodbeck, T., & Reinhardt, V. (1991, October). Perch use by Macaca mulatta in relation to cage location. Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 30(4), 11-12.


Reproduced with permission of Lawrence Erlbaum, publisher of the Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science.

Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 3(2),141-149
©2000


Erratum

In the following sentence [page 114, paragraph 3]

"Schapiro et al. (2000) scored a more than two-fold higher incidence of abnormal behaviors and a more than five-fold higher level of inactivity in six long-tailed macaques housed in relatively dark lower row cages than in three long-tailed macaques housed in relatively bright upper row cages..... "

"... and a more than five-fold higher level of inactivity....." is an erratum.

The correction is:

Inactivity of animals housed in lower row cages was less than
a fifth (0.4±0.7%) of that of animals housed in upper row cages (2.3±2.3%).